Skip to main content

Notice

Please note that most of the software linked on this forum is likely to be safe to use. If you are unsure, feel free to ask in the relevant topics, or send a private message to an administrator or moderator. To help curb the problems of false positives, or in the event that you do find actual malware, you can contribute through the article linked here.
Topic: CINCH cables blind test (Read 70753 times) previous topic - next topic
0 Members and 2 Guests are viewing this topic.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #25
Quote
It would be nice to see a similar test with speaker cables. I suspect that the vast differences in measured parameters (R, L and C) between different cables will have audible effects. It would also be interesting to measure speaker cable parameters, use transmission line theory to calculate the effect they will have on the signal, and see whether listeners report these same differences.
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=297402"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]


Vast differences in L and C might result in audible effects - but the differences of real-world speaker cables are never going to be vast enough. This has been blind and double blind tested many times in the past.

R can make an audible difference when the speaker cable is too long vs the gauge of wire used. The practical effect of the increased R is to decrease the damping factor of the amplifier, potentially to an audible level. Interestingly, listeners sometimes percieve the high resistance cable as sounding better than the low resistance cable. Indeed, it is often found that the low damping factor is what gives tube amplifiers that 'tube sound'. When resistance is added in series with a solid state amplifier to match that of a particular tube amplifier, in many cases it is found that the tube amplifier does not sound audibly different to the solid state amplifier in a double blind listening test.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #26
Very interesting test. Thanks

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #27
Quote
It would be nice to see a similar test with speaker cables. I suspect that the vast differences in measured parameters (R, L and C) between different cables will have audible effects. It would also be interesting to measure speaker cable parameters, use transmission line theory to calculate the effect they will have on the signal, and see whether listeners report these same differences.


There definitely are differences between different speaker cables, but most of them are mainly going back to their R component, influencing the electrical damping factor Q es and thus the behaviour of the bass speaker around the resonance frequency. A High R value will increase Q es, leading to more but less controlled bass.

There are also measurable and audible differences for the mid/hi range, but funny enough you can get rid of most of them by simply making the effort of adding several R-L-C circuits to the crossover of the speaker, in order to get rid of peaks in the impedance curve of the speaker. Especially helmholtz layouts ( bass-reflex ) will react very sensitive to impedance compensation circuits of the 2nd impedance peak ( you can see the resonance frequency of the helmholtz resonator by finding the minimum between the two peaks of the bass speaker ), especially in combination with a low frequency low-pass, like for analog subwoofer or integrated woofer layouts. You can hear the difference directly for voices, as some singers will definitely and immediately loose a couple of pounds of overweight with a simple R-L-C impedance compensation  ....

Christian
matroska project admin

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #28
Quote
Vast differences in L and C might result in audible effects - but the differences of real-world speaker cables are never going to be vast enough. This has been blind and double blind tested many times in the past.
[{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]

The reason I brought it up was that a friend of mine tested a couple of "high end" speaker cables in a local hifi store and found a couple of designs have extremely high C values. If his measurements are correct (will try dig up the raw data and post it here) they suggest that some cables will have an audible effect.
After posting, however, I re-read Rod Elliot's article ([a href="http://sound.westhost.com/cable-z.htm]Loudspeaker Cable Characteristic Impedence[/url]) on the matter. He has made some reliable measurements and calculations. The results presented in the article suggest that even the expensive esoteric crap will not have an audible effect.


CINCH cables blind test

Reply #30
Just wanted to say "thanks" for this test. I hope that, with all the work involved, you also had some fun listening to nice recordings on high quality equipment.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #31
Quote
The agressor signals are electric-magnetic waves. Waves with only a magnetic or only an electric field don't exist. Kill the electric part, kill all.
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=297256"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]

[Slightly off-topic: is there a way to shield audio equipment from cell phone interference? Can this problem be eliminated by using high-end equipment (and, of course, properly shielded cables)?]

Quote
LoL, someone made the horrible mistake of trying to post this over at Head-Fi in the cable forum. 
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=297387"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]

Could you post a link, please? (so we can all have a laugh) 

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #32
Quote
Nice test, i love it.

You could argue the listeners were not trained well enough or had bad ears,


Well, they all passed Phase I -- they all *thought* they heard a difference.  So you'd have to propose that that not passing Phase 2 means, the difference they *thought* they heard wasn't real, but there's still *another* difference they didn't hear.  The thing is, that can always be proposed, as long as results remain null.

Quote
but it certainly proves that any difference, if present, is not at all clear for everyone to hear. A nice additional test might be to setup a high end mic & recording system and analyse the data, it'd be interesting to see if there are inaudible differences between cables. And if so how cheap a cable can be and still perform exactly like the expensive ones.

With a €25k audio setup it doesn't hurt to spend more than €2,30 on audio cables though as you'll want some physical durability and the cost will still be a tiny fraction of the total setup.
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=297099"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]


Or buy/build and ABX box, so that cable switching was faster.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #33
Quote
I think the take home lesson is that, when buying interconnects, go for a decent quality shielded cable with good plugs - anything more than that and you are wasting your money.
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=297402"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]

From my experience, the plugs make a huge difference. I'd always try to have golden plugs because metal plugs tend to corrode. Headphone plugs (6,3mm) are often very bad.

Quote
[Slightly off-topic: is there a way to shield audio equipment from cell phone interference? Can this problem be eliminated by using high-end equipment (and, of course, properly shielded cables)?]
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=297817"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]

The room within a closed metal case is always free from waves like the ones from cell phones. If I recall correctly, then holes in the case must be smaller than the wavelenght of the waves to be excluded. (Please someone confirm or deny ?) E.g. a coaxial cable with infinite lenght and perfect shield (zero resistance) has zero interference. (Faraday-cylinder)
I know that I know nothing. But how can I then know that ?

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #34
The validity of ABX testing for audio equipment is questioned in the following article:
http://www.stereophile.com/news/050905debate/
This article is currently displayed on the home page of the Stereophile website.
Lest there be any question, I am the author.

jason victor serinus

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #35
Quote
The validity of ABX testing for audio equipment is questioned in the following article:
http://www.stereophile.com/news/050905debate/
This article is currently displayed on the home page of the Stereophile website.
Lest there be any question, I am the author.

jason victor serinus
[{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]



Anyone can 'question' something... providing evidence that the questions are valid, is another matter altogether....one that Stereophile hasn't adequately addressed.  The argument Mr. Atkinson presented in the debate would convince no scientist that ABX testing was an invalid method for audio equipment.

Mr. Serinus also wrote the Secrets of Home Theater article onthe power cord test...the results were negative for difference there too (there's an HA thread about it).  He 'questioned' the results there too on several grounds (some valid, some not)...leading his editor to insert a postscript affirming that ABX tests *do* work.

[a href="http://www.hometheaterhifi.com/volume_11_4/feature-article-blind-test-power-cords-12-2004.html]http://www.hometheaterhifi.com/volume_11_4...ds-12-2004.html[/url]

Here is a link to Mr. Serinus' website, describing his work in holistic healing.  I leave it to the reader to judge his ability to evaluate scientific methods:

http://www.planeteria.net/home/whistler/pages/healing.html

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #36
Quote
The validity of ABX testing for audio equipment is questioned in the following article:
http://www.stereophile.com/news/050905debate/
This article is currently displayed on the home page of the Stereophile website.
Lest there be any question, I am the author.

jason victor serinus
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298375"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]


Quoting from your article ...

But tests can neither adequately describe nor convey everything we hear, sense and feel.

That's right ... you don't feel the same way every day so you definitely cannot expect to 'sense' music the same way every day. A shiny, chrome-plated and expensive 'high-end' tube amplifier (with electrical parameters that any japanese electronics engineer would be ashamed of if any of his devices would show these) might sound very pleasant to you on one day ... another day, it might prove to sound 'dirty' whereas the 'boring' jet-black japanese 500$ MOSFET integrated amp might be your favourite of the day ... simply according to the mood you're in.

We all agree on the fact that 'hearing' music must be learned first ... since it is not an ability that you were born with. Thus, concentration abilities will influence your hearing

Objective and scientific blind testing simply rules out your daily mood ... that's why it is so important. Additionally, ABX does not natively allow you to determine which test object sounds 'better' (you'll need something like ABC/HR for that) ... it's just about finding differences on a valid statistical basis without knowing which device is actually playing. The ABX protocol sometimes simply is overrated.

The main problem (to me) that lead to the situation we face today is that HiFi magazines throughout the world are mostly exaggerating on a high level when testing equipment ... describing tonal differences with different speakers is easy ... but whenever they bring up the terms 'sounds more musical' or 'fast sound' (there are literally thousands of these), I need to ask myself what these people are actually writing about.
The name was Plex The Ripper, not Jack The Ripper

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #37
Quote
... but whenever they bring up the terms 'sounds more musical' or 'fast sound' (there are literally thousands of these), I need to ask myself what these people are actually writing about.
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298388"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]

It's the old issue - "sounding better" vs. "sounding more transparent". Most people mix up those two or think its the same, although it is not.

I guess we can all agree that it is just natural that people prefer a certain "coloring" of the sound. Thats an absolutely reasonable and valid wish. The question however is "where" should the coloring happen? Do you want it "hardcoded" into your speakers? Or wouldn't it instead a better idea to demand that the equipment shall sound as transparent as possible, and you then can color the sound on-the-fly with EQs and DSPs?

Playback and recording -equipment should be as transparent as possible - if you want "coloring" then thats the job of "postprocessing" (EQs, DSPs, etc.).

my 2 cents,
- Lyx
I am arrogant and I can afford it because I deliver.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #38
Quote
Or buy/build and ABX box, so that cable switching was faster.
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298154"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]


No, because we are testing the hypothesis that any standard plug or wire will destroy the sound, and that only ultra-expensive ones can transmit the signal unaltered.

Introducing an ABX box with standard interconnects in the path of the signal is a valid procedure only if we first assume that standard interconnects don't spoil the sound, in other words, if we first assume that the tested hypothesis is wrong !

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #39
krabapple:  ad hominem arguments rarely do anything to address the issue at hand.

Jason:  Correct me if I'm wrong on this, but I see three main points in your article raised against the practicality of ABX testing.  I have a response that applies to all of them, but I'd first like to point out each one individually in case I misinterpreted something and you'd like to set the record straight.  The italics are just there to separate what I perceive your arguments to be from my responses; I'm not trying to assign quotes to you.
  • The emotional response to some sections of music is influenced by the music leading up to them and even if the specific sections are not possible to ABX, one piece of equipment may produce a better overall effect than the other.  ABXing the full effect is impossible because the entire thing takes too long for a scientific test.  This one I actually don't have a specific response to.  I can't see how one would prove that just testing clips is fine without comparing a clip test to a test of the entire thing, which I would agree takes entirely too long.
  • Participating in an ABX test places one in circumstances sufficiently different from normal listening that one may lose one's ability to notice subtle differences.  It is true that an ABX test could affect one's listening abilities, but I'm not convinced that this effect would be enough to separate the test from, "normal," listening.  Obviously, if someone went into a test expecting not to hear a difference, then there would be some question to the validity of their test if they came back with a null result, but aside from that one issue, people can be in a variety of emotional states outside of testing, as JeanLuc posted.  If you evaluate one amplifier after getting a promotion at work, then evaluate another after your dog gets run over by a truck, any differences you noted between the two in a convential audiophile magazine writeup would be suspect.  The results of a blind listening test aren't going to be any less reliable than results found outside of one.
  • Music is a very emotional and subjective thing and therefore cannot be properly judged within a scientific test.  I gave a blunt one-line reply to this point in a thread here on HydrogenAudio about the article being referenced.  ABX tests rely on the listener to make the decision as to whether two things sound different, not frequency response graphs or distortion measurements.  Whether the emotional response to music can be measured with a microphone or not is irrelevant to ABX testing because the instruments used in an ABX test are still a very subjective human mind and pair of ears.

Finally, I'd like to say something about ABX testing with regard to buying audio equipment.  When making a purchasing decision, you have to consider the differences that you know you're going to experience outside of audibility, like whether you think one looks more impressive than the other or whether one costs more than the other, along with how different the two pieces of equipment sound.  If you can't ABX two things after several trials, there's at least a good chance that you aren't going to really hear a difference in regular listening.  In that case, I think it would be better to mostly ignore the issue of whether one sounds better than the other when deciding which to buy and focus on how the other things fit together.  Regarding the anecdote John Atkinson gave in the debate about how even though he couldn't ABX two amplifiers, he hated listening to one (I think it might've been a third) and noticed an immediate improvement when switching to another.  In this case, I think it would be valid to say that he enjoyed the other amplifier more, but saying that this is sufficient evidence that the two amplifiers were audibly different is a stretch.  There's nothing to prove that he wouldn't have had a similar change from hate to love if he'd started with the more expensive amplifier and then replaced it with the cheaper one, or that printing off a picture of the more expensive one and taping it to the front of the cheap one wouldn't have made him like it.  There's a lot more uncertainty to subjective testing than ABX testing (hence the term, "subjective") and I think that if someone is trying to decide which of two pieces of audio equipment would be better for them to buy or for them to recommend to others, one should try to remove as much of this uncertainty as possible.

Note added after previewing:  Normally, I try to proofread what I've typed before posting it, but in this case, I think I'm going to make an exception...

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #40
Quote
Quote
Or buy/build and ABX box, so that cable switching was faster.
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298154"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]


No, because we are testing the hypothesis that any standard plug or wire will destroy the sound, and that only ultra-expensive ones can transmit the signal unaltered.

Introducing an ABX box with standard interconnects in the path of the signal is a valid procedure only if we first assume that standard interconnects don't spoil the sound, in other words, if we first assume that the tested hypothesis is wrong !
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298442"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]



Then your DBT will always be open to criticism that it was insensitive to real difference due to the length of the switching interval.

You could instead use the subjective 'best' cable (determined by sighted listening) as the ABX-->preamp cable, and switch between two cables as input.  Or use two inputs in the preamp, from two identical source units, using different cables for each input.

I didn't say it would be easy 

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #41
Quote
krabapple:  ad hominem arguments rarely do anything to address the issue at hand.


Sir, I have addressed the issue at hand numerous times in numerous forums.  And Mr. Serinus quite likely posted here in response to pointers I placed on other audio forums, to this thread, so I feel personally responsible to provide some background.

Personally as a scientist, when confronted with anti-science -- whether it be standard high-end audio journalism, or 'intelligent design' arguments in a Kansas courtroom --  I find it informative to know where the poster is coming from, intellectually...to place a person's claims in some sort of context.  I have  to wonder whether someone who has embraced New Age healing can really be swayed by scientific arguments at all.  I'm also pretty sure Mr. Serinus has encountered them before, on other forums.

Do you think Mr. Serinus' *article*,  if posted here (versus just a link to it) would likely have passed TOS muster?

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #42
Quote
Personally as a scientist, when confronted with anti-science -- whether it be standard high-end audio journalism, or 'intelligent design' arguments in a Kansas courtroom --  I find it informative to know where the poster is coming from, intellectually...to place a person's claims in some sort of context.  I have  to wonder whether someone who has embraced New Age healing can really be swayed by scientific arguments at all.
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298501"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]

While you *may* be right in this case, i dont think one can generalize this. For example, i myself am very interested into spirituality(however, this does not mean that i'm open to any kind of occult BS) and do believe that there is another plane besides of the material one. But i can differentiate when one or the other is useful or efficient. On the topic at hand(and most topics on ha.org) i think the scientific approach is more efficient, because the subjective one would be too error-prone. Engagement in "un-scientific" practices and philosophies does not automatically exclude scientific thinking and abilities. But it can be the case(and often is).

- Lyx
I am arrogant and I can afford it because I deliver.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #43
Quote
Introducing an ABX box with standard interconnects in the path of the signal is a valid procedure only if we first assume that standard interconnects don't spoil the sound, in other words, if we first assume that the tested hypothesis is wrong !
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298442"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]


A proper ABX device does not interfere with the signal directly. Ok, so there may be a few plugs and a bit of wire, but even then you can always test if the ABX switcher alters the sound by testing and switching manually. Once someone realizes that the ABX device does not audibly alter the sound, then they will realize how useful it is for objective comparisons.

Quote
John noted that Arny's PCABX protocol requires that one digitize the output. This introduces an unverified mode of testing and adds variables to the mix, first in the nature of the digitizer, which conceivably (if not indubitably) alters the original sonic data, and second in the unknown nature of the amplifier over which the digital files are played back.


This may be a valid point against PCABX (and subject to manual tests whether it makes an audible difference or not), but not against ABX tests in general.

Quote
He further asserted that while most audiophiles and reviewers audition equipment by listening to familiar music for extended periods of time on one unit, ABX involves listening to short musical snippets of often unfamiliar music on ever-changing units.


Again, you can certainly listen to familiar music for relatively extended periods of time while ABX testing. Yes, a statisitcally relevent number of tests need to be done, but this does not need to be done all at once - the test can be spread over weeks if you wanted to. If you are a music lover, then spending a significant amount of time listening to music doesn't seem to be too much of a demand?

Quote
We need to do what all audiophile reviewers of worth have learned to do: trust our ears.


The best way to trust our ears is to control all of the variables except for the sound itself - that way, any differences that we hear will be real to our ears. So the only way to trust our ears is with ABX testing. Anyone who does not like the objectivity of ABX testing obviously does not trust their ears.
Remember, the ABX procedure itself does not judge the differnece in sound (that is left to the listener), but it enables us to verify if the differences we are hearing are real.

Quote
The crazy thing about this ongoing debate is that listening to music is not a rational, scientific phenomenon. Who knows where the music that a composer puts on paper actually comes from, let alone the source of the inspiration with which musicians bring those notes to life? Can you explain why one interpretation of a classic song moves you to tears and another leaves you cold? Can you scientifically explain what makes your heart skip a beat?

Of course not. And since audio components and cables are nothing more than electrical conduits for that which lies beyond words and reason, how can we expect charts, graphs, and short bursts of preselected tones to tell us everything about how a component will affect us, let alone how it will interact with other components in a given listening environment?

One thing is certain: Music is music, and protocols are protocols. ABX may be a valid testing protocol for laboratory equipment, but when you bring auditory sensation, emotional reaction, and other, at best, only partially understood mechanisms of brain response into the equation, you are pretending that you can objectively test that which you cannot adequately explain. And please, please don't try to convince me that because you can't fully explain something or detect it with scientific instruments, it doesn't exist. Better to sell me the Brooklyn Bridge.


This is precisely why ABX testing is important. ABX lets us verify that the supposed audible differences between two components is real or not, without letting any other variables (some of which we may not yet understand) interfere with the result.

PS, many of the questions posed in the above quote are in fact being studied by scientists...

The article is obviously biased in that it keeps mentioning the word "absolute" to sway the reader. It is clear that the author is missing the point of science and objective testing. Science is not about absolute certanty, it is about highly probable outcomes. As science progresses, scientists develop more and more probable (in terms of accuracy) theories. For a theory to be of high probability, it has to be consistent with all scientific observations. The scientific method is used to control all of the variables except for the one that you are trying to test - so there is a high probability of the cause leading to the effect.

A theory does not have to be absolutely correct for it to be useful in the real world. Using a highly probable theory is far more useful than a theory with a much lower probability.

ABX should be used to determine whether there actually is an audible difference between two components, as the results of an ABX test are far more likely to be correct than a single sighted AB comparison.

But if you believe that there is no difference between your (potentially flawed) perceptions and reality, then I believe there is an opening for you in the Oceania outer party. 

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #44
Besides all this discussion, there's a thing that Pio's test clearly proves and that was already well known: that our perception and reaction to sound is easily influenced by our mind even in unpredictable ways. In other words, we (I guess some people more than other) easily tend to hear differences where there are none. Remember, in this test, people really heard differences. But those differences didn't exist in reality. It was just their mood, subsconcious bias, beliefs, or other factors , that made them feel that  some supposed cable sounded better than other.

But high-end proponents always ignore this basic fact as a key factor for invalidating their way of evaluating audio equipment. Blind tests, even if they had shortcomings (which is up to discussion), are so far the most realiable way of analyzing real differences in sound.

Said this, I think there's nothing against people buying equipment that makes them feel the music better, even when that enjoyment comes more from the placebo-effect side of the equipment than the actual sound they produce. Of course, in a serious discussion about these issues, both effects should be acknowledged and analyzed by separate.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #45
Quote
Then your DBT will always be open to criticism that it was insensitive to real difference due to the length of the switching interval.


I adress this issue with phase 1 and 2.
In phase 1, listeners look for an audible difference. They decide the switching interval so that the difference is audible.
In phase 2, they check that this difference is real, before starting the test.

Quote
You could instead use the subjective 'best' cable (determined by sighted listening) as the ABX-->preamp cable, and switch between two cables as input.  Or use two inputs in the preamp, from two identical source units, using different cables for each input.

I didn't say it would be easy 
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298499"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]


Using a cable for the ABX->preamp interconnect is not a good solution. The ABX switcher can mask differences between the cables.
Using two identical sources might be a solution. However it was criticized because the cables would interfere with each other. Some cables are designed to kill stationary waves caused by external influences. If they are compared to standard cables plugged into the same device, the standard cable can receive that radiofrequency and inject it into the other cable.

Quote
A proper ABX device does not interfere with the signal directly.


Then ABXing cables makes no sense.

Quote
you can always test if the ABX switcher alters the sound by testing and switching manually. Once someone realizes that the ABX device does not audibly alter the sound, then they will realize how useful it is for objective comparisons.


I'm pretty sure that any hardcore audiophile would "hear" the switching box altering the sound. You know, I've been told over and over by all audiophiles that it was impossible to listen to a hifi system before buying it if a switch was in the path of the signal.
In order to prove that the ABX switch is transparent, we need to show that it is not ABXable itself. Thus we need to run an ABX test where it will be plugged and unplugged between each playback. Since this is unavoidable, let's save time and apply directly this procedure to the cables.

But actually, it's worse than this : two people already stressed that the 1500 € Marantz SACD player was too poor to reveal the sonic differences between the cables, and that we should have used the Acuphase one instead.

That's why the real solution is to invite hardcore audiophiles to take part to phase 1. We were no hardcore audiophiles, and our first reaction, at the beginning of phase 1, was that all the cables sounded the same. I must admit that the differences that we believed to hear after this in phase 1 and 2 were only caused by our desire to hear something altogether.

This wouldn't spoil statistics. Phase 3 has not started yet. Technically, the test is still running in phase 2. And jumping between phases 1, 2 and 3 is not forbidden as long as results given in phase 3 are permanent, and the number of trials and listeners of phase 3 are not changed after one result of phase 3 at least is known.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #46
Quote
Quote
Then your DBT will always be open to criticism that it was insensitive to real difference due to the length of the switching interval.


I adress this issue with phase 1 and 2.
In phase 1, listeners look for an audible difference. They decide the switching interval so that the difference is audible.
In phase 2, they check that this difference is real, before starting the test.



Yes, but one could be *really picky*, and suggest that you havent' ruled out that there is still a *real difference* that went undetected, because of the switching interval effect.  Certainly in an actual scientific experiment, there would be an attempt to reduce the switching intervals to be essentially instantaneous, to avoid this sort of criticism.  It's tedious but necessary if you want to make the case airtight.


Quote
Quote
You could instead use the subjective 'best' cable (determined by sighted listening) as the ABX-->preamp cable, and switch between two cables as input.  Or use two inputs in the preamp, from two identical source units, using different cables for each input.

I didn't say it would be easy 
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298499"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]


Using a cable for the ABX->preamp interconnect is not a good solution. The ABX switcher can mask differences between the cables.


LOL.  In terms of pickiness, that's the same sort of objection as I raise above.

Quote
Using two identical sources might be a solution. However it was criticized because the cables would interfere with each other. Some cables are designed to kill stationary waves caused by external influences. If they are compared to standard cables plugged into the same device, the standard cable can receive that radiofrequency and inject it into the other cable.


And again..

Certainly the switching interval effect (the result of limits on audible memory) is *real* and has been documented, while these other effects seen speculative.  So why are you worried about one and not the other?


Quote
Quote
A proper ABX device does not interfere with the signal directly.


Then ABXing cables makes no sense.


? A properly designed passive switch should have no sonic effect on the inputs and outputs, other than the switching.


Quote
Quote
you can always test if the ABX switcher alters the sound by testing and switching manually. Once someone realizes that the ABX device does not audibly alter the sound, then they will realize how useful it is for objective comparisons.


I'm pretty sure that any hardcore audiophile would "hear" the switching box altering the sound. You know, I've been told over and over by all audiophiles that it was impossible to listen to a hifi system before buying it if a switch was in the path of the signal.
In order to prove that the ABX switch is transparent, we need to show that it is not ABXable itself. Thus we need to run an ABX test where it will be plugged and unplugged between each playback. Since this is unavoidable, let's save time and apply directly this procedure to the cables.

But actually, it's worse than this : two people already stressed that the 1500 € Marantz SACD player was too poor to reveal the sonic differences between the cables, and that we should have used the Acuphase one instead.



You're surprised by such a response?   

Quote
That's why the real solution is to invite hardcore audiophiles to take part to phase 1. We were no hardcore audiophiles, and our first reaction, at the beginning of phase 1, was that all the cables sounded the same. I must admit that the differences that we believed to hear after this in phase 1 and 2 were only caused by our desire to hear something altogether.

This wouldn't spoil statistics. Phase 3 has not started yet. Technically, the test is still running in phase 2. And jumping between phases 1, 2 and 3 is not forbidden as long as results given in phase 3 are permanent, and the number of trials and listeners of phase 3 are not changed after one result of phase 3 at least is known.
[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298560"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]


Pio, I think your test was great.  But now you know the sorts of quibbles that you can expect to encounter.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #47
Quote
Yes, but one could be *really picky*, and suggest that you havent' ruled out that there is still a *real difference* that went undetected


That's right. However, the differences ruled out are larger and larger.
We showed that some skeptics (Emmanuel Piat and me), and some unconvinced people (Marsupilami and JC07) could be lured into hearing things if they force themselves to look for differences between sources that seem identical to them at first sight.
That's completely different than proving that someone absolutely certain to pass the test because differences are so huge that they can't be missed, can fail.

Quote
Certainly in an actual scientific experiment, there would be an attempt to reduce the switching intervals to be essentially instantaneous, to avoid this sort of criticism.


I didn't get this critisism at all. 99% of audiophile people acknowledge that all cable sound the same when switched quickly (Edit : interconnect cables without RLC filters). There is no debate about this fact.  One of the main criticism was exactly the opposite : the listening sessions were too short ! The majority of audiophile who believe in cables say that the differences can only be heard after a long time, usually several days of use (not burn-in, but just hearing adaptation from the listener).
A long-term test have already been proposed with listening sessions of one week each in the workgroup I created in homecinema-fr.com. The listener accept to have his cables hidden at home. Someone visits him, and is left alone in the room. He does something, or not. The listener can listen during the whole next week to decide if the cable have been changed. I'll be able to provide the statistic analysis for them if they find some listeners to start the test.

I know that scientifically, this kind of test have near 100 % of chances to fail, but that's not what audiophile believe.

Quote
Certainly the switching interval effect (the result of limits on audible memory) is *real* and has been documented, while these other effects seen speculative.  So why are you worried about one and not the other?


As I said, it spoils the test, because obvioulsy, one cannot prove somthing if he assumes that this thing is wrong in the hypothesis on which his demonstration is based.

When I test something, I must have the decency to read the instruction manual. For exemple, the cables tested had a direction. Audiophiles say that these cables, plugged in the right direction, sound better than standard ones. I want to see if this is right or wrong. Not if audiophile cables plugged in the wrong direction sound better than stadard ones. Audiophile never said that expensive cables sounded better than standard ones in the wrong direction. There is no need to test it. Everyone agrees on this.

In the same way, no audiophile pretends that high end cable sound better than standard cables when an ABX switcher is plugged in the middle of the path of the signal. I'm not going to test this. All I want to test is audiophile claims, and this is something that no audiophile claims.
Actually, it is very probable that the cable manufacturer himself would dismiss that use of his cable. Testing a device, I must use it as it is intended to be used. Otherwise I'm not testing its claimed properties.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #48
Quote
Sir, I have addressed the issue at hand numerous times in numerous forums.[a href="index.php?act=findpost&pid=298501"][{POST_SNAPBACK}][/a]
I didn't mean to suggest that you haven't.

Quote
And Mr. Serinus quite likely posted here in response to pointers I placed on other audio forums, to this thread, so I feel personally responsible to provide some background.

Personally as a scientist, when confronted with anti-science -- whether it be standard high-end audio journalism, or 'intelligent design' arguments in a Kansas courtroom --  I find it informative to know where the poster is coming from, intellectually...to place a person's claims in some sort of context.  I have  to wonder whether someone who has embraced New Age healing can really be swayed by scientific arguments at all.  I'm also pretty sure Mr. Serinus has encountered them before, on other forums.
If the arguments he presented in the article were new ideas that he and maybe a few other people were coming out with, then I would agree that his background would be something that should be considered; however, since I've heard everything he said in that article from other people advocating subjective testing over ABX testing, I don't think questioning his background is a good way to meet his objections.  If he followed you here from some other board where you gave direct responses to all of his claims, well, then I'm sorry for singling out your one post.

Quote
Do you think Mr. Serinus' *article*,  if posted here (versus just a link to it) would likely have passed TOS muster?
No.

CINCH cables blind test

Reply #49
i use just some Composite Video cables that came bundels with some GFX card at my work.
Its same plugs as the normap phono cable, but now left/right are seperate to differet cables and there is some kind of shielding.

but i only do this because they where free  :-)